Body-cams: Solution to police-civilian violence?

Print Friendly
Body-cams: Solution to police-civilian violence?
A uniformed police officer holds his body-worn camera for a photo-op.
A Minnesota police officer sports a chest-mounted body camera.
Photo: crayfisher

New York City is in turmoil in the wake of the July 17, 2014, death of Eric Garner at the hands of city police. On Dec. 3, demonstrations broke out after a grand jury declined to prosecute the officers. Then, on Dec. 20, two police officers were murdered in their car by a 28-year-old with a long criminal record as vengeance for Garner’s death. At the funerals, police have turned their backs on Mayor Bill de Blasio.

The death of Michael Brown, 18, from police gunshots in Ferguson, Mo., on Aug. 9, had already highlighted the outsize death toll among African-Americans in “police-involved shootings.”

Could police body-cameras, which record tense meet-ups between police and civilians, avert some confrontations and improve behavior on both sides? President Obama, for one, has advocated much wider use to reduce deaths among unarmed civilians.

Cameras seem promising, but remember that Garner’s death only aroused such a furious reaction because his last minutes were captured on a bystander’s cell-phone video. Despite this frightening footage — and even after the medical examiner labeled Garner’s death “homicide” — the district attorney declined to prosecute the police who took him down in a chokehold.

Photograph of several men and women filling an urban street while marching and wielding protest signs.
Protesters march against police brutality in New York City on Dec 13th, 2014.

As the protests and counter-protests continued in New York, we noticed a new, controlled study1 showing that body-cameras reduced the use of force and complaints against the police in Railto, Calif.

Body-cams are only the latest type of camera to be used in policing, and as we set out to explore the science of the video surveillance devices used in law enforcement, we realized why the California study was so noteworthy: Scientific studies on cameras in law enforcement are scarce. Indeed, A 2014 study2 of body-cams concluded that, “The overwhelming theme from this review is the lack of available research on the technology.”

And that’s doubly true for studies published in peer-reviewed journals.

What’s a camera good for?

The use of body-cams, car-cams and stationary cameras to enforce laws and document police behavior are justified as a means to:

deter crime

provide better evidence for prosecution

moderate aggressive behavior of civilians and officers alike

defend police against unfounded accusations of brutality

monitor officer behavior to ensure accountability and professional standards

Cameras may simply remind both sides that evidence of any wayward behavior will be gathered, but they can also invoke a mechanism called “social surveillance.” This is an idea from social psychology that people are more likely to follow behavioral norms — to act as they should — when someone is watching.

So what do we know about cops and cameras?

A 3-D graph showing the numbers of police use-of-force and public complaints from 2009 to 2013. Both categories decreased following the introduction of body-worn cameras to the police force.
Rialto police recorded more than 43,000 contacts with the public per year, with the year of the body-cam experiment resulting in fewer accounts of use-of-force and filed complaints. Notice this occurred for officers with and without cameras.
Adapted from Barak Ariel et al. 3

2014: Body-cam study shows benefits, raises more questions

The new study just mentioned, touted as the first controlled experiment of body-cams, ran for one year in Railto, a city of 100,000 east of Los Angeles.

The 115 police officers in Railto work 54 shifts per week; during the experiment, half of the shifts wore HD body-cams, and the other half did not. The experiment had the cooperation of police chief William Farrar, a co-author of the study.

For organizational and statistical reasons, each officer took part in both the control and experimental wings, meaning they wore the cams on some shifts, but not others.

Officers were instructed to “film” during any interaction with the public aside from interactions with informants or related to a sexual assault on a minor. The cameras were visible, but officers were also told to announce their presence when an interaction with the public began. At the end of each shift, audio and color video was downloaded into a web-based evidence management system.

The study defined use of force as involving pepper spray, baton, Taser, dog bite or gunshot — anything stronger than a “compliance hold” (a wrestling technique that causes pain at a joint or pressure point). The researchers focused on the use of force, and neither measured the amount of force nor whether it was justified.

The results were almost too good to be true: compared to the prior year, use of force declined by 58 percent. Citizen complaints dropped by 88 percent.

Dash cam video shows a Texas police officer using a stun gun on a 76-year-old man during a routine traffic stop. The officer was later fired for breach of department conduct and policies. KDVR (Denver)

Those plunging numbers, however, beg for explanation, as serious declines appeared in both the camera-equipped and camera-free shifts. The study authors proposed “a spillover effect” as an explanation. “The effect of being observed during experimental shifts diffused to control [non-camera] shifts,” they wrote. By modifying “the perception of individuals about what socially acceptable behavior should be in police-public interactions,” even officers who were not wearing a camera “changed their responses.”

Evidence in criminology “is clearly suggestive of social processes that could explain the reduction in use of force beyond the manipulation,” they wrote. “Peer pressure, social desirability, deterrence, leadership, perceptions of danger and crowd influence … could lead officers to control or not control themselves.”

Despite at least one emailed interview request to each of the three authors, none responded. But it’s fair to say that even before their study was published in the scientific journal, it was being used to justify wider police use of body-cams.

2001: In-car cameras

The new study added to a scanty record of study of cameras, crime, policing and police behavior. We located a study4 of dash cameras on state police cars, written in 2001, when the cameras were already mounted in 72 percent of state-police patrol cars.

The study, based on site visits, surveys and focus groups, found that

33 percent of officers reported that cameras caused them to feel safer on the job

48 percent “reported that citizens have become less aggressive after learning the event was being recorded”

70 percent reported that the cameras had little or no affect on their job performance

93 percent said video evidence from dash cams had exonerated an officer after a civilian complaint

Photo of a building roof with three rectangular cameras fixed in different vantage points
Surveillance cameras on the corner of a building. Their effectiveness depends on placement — and on the boredom and sleepiness of whoever is monitoring them!
Photo: Hustvedt

2009: Stationary video cameras

Mounted cameras — the kind that helped finger two suspects in the Boston Marathon bombing — have become a fact of life in cities. A 2009 analysis5 of 41 systems, mainly in the United Kingdom, but also in the United States, Sweden and elsewhere, found a modest, 16 percent reduction in crime, with almost all of the improvement coming from a 51 percent reduction in six “car parks” (parking lots) in the United Kingdom.

20 studies in city or town centers produced a non-significant, 7 percent reduction in crime

Nine studies in public housing also produced a 7 percent, non-significant reduction in crime

Fourteen of the 15 studies that showed some benefit were in the U.K., a trend-setter in city-wide surveillance

A district attorney shows video of officers checking the ID of a homeless man. When he pulls a knife on them, they give chase and eventually shoot him. The DA explains why the officers are not charged, based partly on the video evidence.
Video: Lancaster Newspapers, Inc.

2011: The three-cities study

The first hurdle in evaluating a surveillance system is surprisingly tough: choosing a basis for comparison. Another section of the same city (or a similar part of a different city) may have different rates of crime or aggressive behavior by police or civilians. If you compare the same area before and after the cameras are connected when the background crime rate is changing, how much of the effect you see is really attributable to the cameras?

With those concerns in mind, what does a 2011 study6 tell us about surveillance cameras installed during the mid-2000s in Chicago, Baltimore and Washington, D.C.?

After more than 50 cameras were installed in downtown Baltimore, violent crime went down 23 percent, and total crime, down 25 percent. However, crime also declined 30 percent in areas within 1,000 feet of the cameras. (Neighboring areas are always of concern due to fear that the cameras may just budge crime sideways without actually reducing it.) Furthermore, the 35 percent decrease in on-street larceny started one year after camera installation. If cameras caused this effect, why the delay?

In the Greenmount section of Baltimore, a 20 percent decline in total crime resulted in eight fewer number of crimes in the average month. In the Humboldt Park section of Chicago, cameras were associated with a nearly one-third reduction in robberies and drug-related crimes. In contrast, a study of cameras in West Garfield Park, Chicago, “yielded little to report.” And a camera system in Washington, D.C., “did not appear to have an impact on crime.”

 Time series graph of crime count in downtown Baltimore from 2003 to 2008 showing the reduction in crime after the installation of security cameras in 2005.
The installation of 50 on-street cameras resulted in a significant drop in crime in downtown Baltimore.
p. 31, 7

Start making sense?

Photo of a uniformed man sitting behind a semi-circular desk of several monitors; a wall of large monitors with security footage is visible in the background.
Few eyes, many screens. British highway patrolmen survey countless monitors fed by security footage across Britain’s roadways.

Ubiquitous video cameras have changed policing as they have changed other realms of modern life. While recent events have placed the focus on preventing abuse of authority, the original motivation was to prevent crime or aid the prosecution.

For fixed cameras, used for remote monitoring of streets, parking lots and buildings, both the density of cameras and the intensity of monitoring matter. The location of monitoring sites and the identity of monitors may be regulated to allay privacy concerns, but excess restrictions can obviate benefits. In the ineffective Washington system, for example, all camera feeds went to a central office, where video feeds could only be monitored when a police lieutenant was present. Police departments have much wider access in other systems, which may increase the chance of noticing aberrant behavior.

Monitoring can be boring, and when the person watching the monitors is inattentive or sleepy, the result may be a false sense of security.

Policy decisions also matter in the acquisition and use of video evidence:

when car- and body-cams must be running

penalties for turning cameras off

where and how long video files must be retained

Finally, consider the “CSI effect.” Prosecutors worry that once cameras become widespread, judges and juries will begin to expect a video record of crimes, and will subconsciously downgrade other forms of evidence in favor of whatever can be verified via video.

– David J. Tenenbaum

8 9 10 11 12

Kevin Barrett, project assistant; Terry Devitt, editor; S.V. Medaris, designer/illustrator; David J. Tenenbaum, feature writer

Bibliography

  1. The effect of police body-worn cameras on use-of-force and citizens’ complaints against the police: A randomized, controlled trial, Barak Ariel et al, Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 2014.
  2. White, Michael D., Aug. 2014. Police Officer Body-Worn Cameras: Assessing the Evidence. Washington, DC; “Office of Community Oriented Policing Services
  3. The effect of police body-worn cameras on use-of-force and citizens’ complaints against the police: A randomized, controlled trial, Barak Ariel et al, Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 2014.
  4. A National Study on the Use and Impact of In-Car Cameras, International Association of Chiefs of Police, In-Car Camera Project, 2001.
  5. Public Area CCTV and Crime Prevention: An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis, Brandon C. Welsh and David P. Farrington, Justice Quarterly, Vol. 26 #4 (December 2009)
  6. Evaluating the Use of Public Surveillance Cameras for Crime Control and Prevention, Nancy G. La Vigne et al, Urban Institute, Sept. 2011
  7. Evaluating the Use of Public Surveillance Cameras for Crime Control and Prevention, Nancy G. La Vigne et al, Urban Institute, Sept. 2011.
  8. Viewer discretion advised: Body-cam footage of Idaho police officer shooting man with knife…
  9. Suicide by cop in Ohio
  10. UK and US watchdogs warn of surveillance risks and the impact on society.
  11. Los Angeles police order Tasers that activate body cameras.
  12. The NYPD officers who see racial bias in the NYPD.